Statistics for Decision Makers - 06.04 - Research Design - Experimental Design

From Training Material
Jump to navigation Jump to search


title
06.04 - Research Design - Experimental Design
author
Bernard Szlachta (NobleProg Ltd) bs@nobleprog.co.uk

Goals。

  • There are many ways an experiment can be designed.
  • For example, subjects can all be tested under each of the treatment conditions or a different group of subjects can be used for each treatment.
  • An experiment might have just one independent variable or it might have several.
  • The goal is to be able to describe basic experimental designs and their advantages, disadvantages and workarounds.


Between-Subjects Designs。

  • Similar to A/B design
  • The various experimental treatments are given to different groups of subjects.
Other names

Between-group Design

Web Interface Ratings Example。

  • The subjects were randomly divided into two groups.
  • The first group used the web application with the first interface design. The second group used the second interface design.
  • The independent variable is "Condition" and has two levels (Design 1 and Design 2).
  • It is a between-subjects variable because different subjects were used for the two levels of the independent variable: the "Design 1" or the "Design 2" condition.
  • The comparison of the Design 1 condition with the Design 2 condition is a comparison between the subjects in one condition with the subjects in the other condition.

Web Interface Ratings Example - chance differences 。

  • The two conditions were treated exactly the same except for the interface design.
  • Any differences between conditions should be attributed to the interface designs themselves.
  • The possibility of chance differences between the groups is ignored.
  • That is, by chance, the raters in one condition might have, on average, been more lenient than the raters in the other condition.
  • Randomly assigning subjects to treatments:
    • ensures that all differences between conditions are chance differences;
    • it does not ensure there will be no differences.

Web Interface Ratings Example - estimating chance differences 。

How to distinguish real differences from chance differences?

  • Analyzing the data from this experiment reveals that the ratings in the Design 1 condition were higher than those in the Design 2 condition.
  • Using inferential statistics, it can be calculated that the probability of finding a difference as large or larger than the one obtained if the treatment had no effect is only 0.018.
  • Therefore it seems likely that the design had an effect and it is not the case that all differences were chance differences.

Independent variables。

Independent variables often have several levels

Example
  • A company tries to determine whether the colour of the logo changes the perception of the trustworthiness of the company.
  • The independent variable is "colour" and there are four levels of this independent variable:

ClipCapIt-140530-155020.PNG

1. blue

2. red

3. green

4. yellow

  • Keep in mind that although there are four levels, there is only one independent variable.
  • Designs with more than one independent variable are considered next.

Multi-Factor Between-Subject Designs。

  • In the "Bias Against Associates of the Obese" experiment, the qualifications of potential job applicants were judged.
  • Each applicant was accompanied by an associate.
  • The experiment had two independent variables:
    1. The weight of the associate (obese or average).
    2. The applicant's relationship to the associate (girlfriend or acquaintance).

ClipCapIt-140610-035530.PNG

Multi-Factor Between-Subject Designs。

ClipCapIt-140610-035530.PNG

This design can be described as a

Weight (2) x  Relationship (2) factorial design
  • The dependent variable was a rating of the applicant's qualifications (on a 9-point scale).
Alternatives
  • If two separate experiments had been conducted to test:
    1. The effect of Associate's Weight and
    2. The effect of Associate's Relationship
  • Then there would be no way to assess whether the effect of Associate's Weight depended on the Associate's Relationship.
Interactions
  • One might imagine that the Associate's Weight would have a larger effect if the associate were a girlfriend rather than merely an acquaintance.
  • A factorial design allows this question to be addressed.
  • When the effect of one variable does differ depending on the level of the other variable then it is said that there is an interaction between the variables.

Within-Subjects Designs。

  • A within-subjects design differs from a between-subjects design in that the same subjects perform at all levels of the independent variable.
  • AKA: repeated-measures designs


Counterbalancing。

  • In a within-subject design it is important not to confound the order in which a task is performed with the experimental treatment.
  • For example, people presented with UI Design 2 tend to asses it worse when presented with UI Design 1 first.
First test Second test
Subject1 Test A Test B
Subject2 Test B Test A
Subject3 Test A Test B
Subject4 Test B Test A
... ... ...

Counterbalancing。

  • Counterbalancing is not a satisfactory solution if there are complex dependencies between:
    • which treatment precedes which
    • the dependent variable.
  • In these cases, it is usually better to use a between-subjects design than a within-subjects design.

Advantage of Within-Subjects Designs。

Individual differences in subjects' overall levels of performance are controlled.
  • Subjects will invariably differ greatly from one another.
  • In an experiment on problem solving, some subjects will be better than others regardless of the condition they are in.
  • Within-subjects designs control these individual differences by comparing the scores of a subject in one condition to the scores of the same subject in other conditions.
  • Each subject serves as his or her own "control".
Power
  • Typically within-subjects designs are more powerful than between-subjects designs.
  • Within-subjects designs are more able to detect an effect than are between-subjects designs.
ClipCapIt-140610-042046.PNG

Alternative Names of Within-Subjects Designs

  • Within-subject designs are often called repeated-measures designs since repeated measurements are taken for each subject.
  • Similarly, a within-subject variable can be called a repeated-measures factor.

Complex Designs。

Designs can contain combinations of between-subject and within-subject variables.

Example
  • Between-subject variable:
    • Gender.
  • Within-subject variables:
    • The type of user interface design.
    • The type of graphic design.

Control Group。

  • A statistician's wife had twins.
  • The statistician rang the minister
  • "Bring them to church on Sunday and we'll baptize them," said the minister.
  • "No," replied the statistician. "Baptize one. We'll keep the other as a control."
STATS: The Magazine For Students of Statistics, Winter 1996, Number 15

Control Group。

  • If A occurs than B occurs (Treatment Group).
  • If A doesn't occur than B doesn't occur (Control Group).
Example
  • Half of the people visiting the website will use the old user interface design, half will use the new one.
  • This will prevent speculation that the time spent on the website would increase even if we did not change the user interface.

Quiz。

Please find the quiz here

Quiz

<quiz display=simple>

{Fifty subjects are each tested in both a control condition and an experimental condition. This is an example of:

|type="()"} - A between-subjects design. + A within-subjects variable.

{

Answer >>

A within-subjects variable

This is a within-subjects design since each subject was tested in each condition.

}

{The time it took each of 20 subjects to name a set of coloured squares and to read a set of colour names was recorded. This is an example of:

|type="()"} - A between-subjects design. + A within-subjects variable.

{

Answer >>

A within-subjects variable

This is a within-subjects design since each subject was tested in each condition.

}

{Subjects are randomly assigned to either a drug condition or a placebo condition. This is an example of:

|type="()"} + A between-subjects design. - A within-subjects variable.

{

Answer >>

A between-subjects design

This is a between-subjects design because each subject was tested in either one condition (drug) or another (placebo).

}

{For which of the following is there an interaction?

|type="[]"} + A: The effect of the treatment was larger for women than for men. - B: For both women and men the treatment had a large effect. + C: The combination of smoking and drinking increases the chances of throat cancer more than one would expect from the effects of smoking and drinking considered alone.

{

Answer >>

A and C

}